The Role of Music 14
14
Research has shown that the beneficial effects of asynchronous music are reduced once
exercise intensity exceeds the anaerobic threshold. For example, using the Wingate test (a
maximal cycle ergometer effort over 30 s), Pujol and Langenfeld (1999) found that music had
no positive influence on performance; a finding that supports the load-dependent hypothesis.
In a subsequent study using a treadmill and outdoor running task at 90% VO
2
max
[A DOT
ABOVE THE V IS REQUIRED]
, Tenenbaum et al. (2004) showed that while motivational
asynchronous music did shape interpretations of
fatigue symptoms, it did not reduce
perceptions of effort.
Tempo is generally considered to be a key determinant of the aesthetic response to a piece
of music and is an important musical constituent to consider when prescribing music for
exercise (Crust, 2008; Edworthy & Waring, 2006; Karageorghis & Terry, 1997).
Accordingly, a recent series of studies led by the second author has investigated the
relationship between exercise heart rate and preference for music tempo. Previous
investigations by Iwanaga (1995a, 1995b) had suggested a positive and linear relationship
between heart rate and music-tempo preference. However, methodological limitations
inherent
in these studies, such as lack of a heart rate manipulation and self-regulation of
music tempo, detracted from the ecological validity of the findings. Karageorghis and his
collaborators sought to re-examine Iwanaga‟s findings having strengthened the methods used.
In the first study, participants reported their preference for slow (80 bpm), medium (120
bpm) and fast (140 bpm) tempo music selections while walking on a treadmill at 40%, 60%,
and 75% of maximal heart rate reserve (maxHRR). Participants expressed a strong preference
for fast and medium tempo music over slow music regardless of work intensity. Also, an
interaction effect was found, whereby participants reported a preference for either fast or
medium tempo music during low and moderate exercise intensities,
but preferred fast tempo
music during high intensity exercise (Karageorghis, Jones, & Low, 2006).
The Role of Music 15
15
Karageorghis, Jones, and Stuart (2008) extended this approach to study the psychological
impact of entire music programs, rather than just excerpts of music. They administered
medium tempi, fast tempi, mixed tempi (tracks arranged in the order medium-fast-fast-
medium-fast-fast) conditions and a no-music control condition, while participants worked at
70% maxHRR on a treadmill. Dependent
measures were music preference, intrinsic
motivation, and global flow. The researchers expected the mixed-tempi condition to yield the
most positive psychological effects because it was well aligned with the selected work
intensity, and there was also less likelihood of participants experiencing boredom or
irritation. However, findings showed that it was actually the medium-tempi condition that
elicited the most positive psychological outcomes.
Based on the findings of their first two studies, Karageorghis and colleagues suggested
that there may be a step change in preference between 70% and 75% maxHRR, reflected by a
stronger preference for fast tempi music at the higher heart rate. The step
change in
preference parallels the anaerobic threshold, which is characterized by a greater reliance on
anaerobic pathways for energy production. Moreover, at these moderate-to-high intensities
participants become more acutely aware of fatigue-related cues (cf. Rejeski, 1985).
These findings led us to question the positive linear relationship between heart rate and
preferred music tempo that Iwanaga had proposed (Karageorghis & Terry, 2009). We
hypothesized that the relationship between these variables is quartic in nature, with three
inflection points (see Figure 17.4). During the early stages
of an exercise bout, when RPE is
low, the relationship is linear, whereas at moderate-to-high exercise intensities, music of fast
and medium tempi is preferred equally. Beyond 70% maxHRR, fast tempi are preferred and
the linearity of the relationship resumes. Once exercise intensity exceeds 80% maxHRR,
there is a “ceiling effect” for tempo preference. The reason for this is that relatively few
tracks are recorded at very high tempi and so our band of most familiar tempi is 80-140 bpm.
The Role of Music 16
16
Also, given the automatic attentional switching that occurs at high exercise intensities, it is
unlikely that music of any tempo can be attended to selectively (Hernandez-Peon, 1961;
Rejeski, 1985). In
other words, silence
is
possibly “golden” during very high intensity
exercise. This notion does, however, require further empirical investigation.
We recently tested the hypothesized quartic relationship between exercise heart rate and
music tempo preference (Karageorghis, Jones, Priest et al., in press) using six exercise
intensities (40-90% maxHRR) and four music tempi (slow, medium, fast, very fast). Results
generally supported a quartic relationship between heart rate and preferred music tempo,
although in
the early stages of exercise, the preference was for music of a higher tempo than
we had predicted. Moreover, between 70% and 80% of maxHRR, rather than a “ceiling
effect”, we observed a dip in music tempo preference. This has been termed the “Clarke dip”
after a precocious undergraduate, Adam Clarke, who predicted it during the early stages of
the research project, despite the second author‟s forecast to the contrary.
______________________
Insert Figure 17.4
______________________
Do'stlaringiz bilan baham: