Ahrens, Kathleen. 1995. The mental representation of verbs. Ph.D. diss., University of California at San
Diego.
———. 2003. Verbal Integration: The interaction of participant roles and sentential argument structure.
Journal of Psycholinguistic Research
22:497–516.
Anderson, Steven. R. 1971. On the role of deep structure in semantic interpretation.
Foundations of Lan-
guage
6:197–219.
Anh, Woo-Kyong, and Douglas L. Medin. 1992. A two-stage model of category construction.
Cognitive
Science
16:81–121.
Bates, Elizabeth, and Judith Goodman. 1997. On the inseparability of grammar and the lexicon: Evidence
from acquisition, aphasia, and real time processing.
Language and Cognitive Processes
12, nos. 5–
6:507–84.
Bencini, Giulia M. L. 2002. The representation and processing of argument structure constructions. Ph.D.
diss., University of Illinois.
Bencini, Giulia, and Adele Goldberg. 2000. The contribution of argument structure constructions to sen-
tence meaning.
Journal of Memory and Language
43:640–51.
Bencini, Giulia, Katheryn J. Bock, and Adele E. Goldberg. n.d. How abstract is grammar? Unpublished
manuscript.
Bock, Kathryn. 1986. Syntactic persistence in language production.
Cognitive Psychology
18:355–87.
———. 1995. Sentence production: From mind to mouth. In
Handbook of perception and cognition:
Speech, language and communication,
vol. 11, ed. Joanne L. Miller and Peter D. Eimas, 181–216.
Orlando, Fla.: Academic Press.
Bock, Kathryn, and Zenzi M. Griffin. 2000. The persistence of structural priming: Transient activation or
implicit learning?
Journal of Experimental Psychology-General
129:177–92.
Bock, Kathryn, and Wilhelm Levelt. 1994. Language production: Grammatical encoding. In
Handbook of
psycholinguistics,
ed. Morton Ann Gernsbacher, 945–84. San Diego: Academic Press.
Bock, Kathryn, and Helga Loebell. 1990. Framing sentences.
Cognition
35:1–39.
Bock, Kathryn, Helga Loebell, and Randal Morey. 1992. From conceptual roles to structural relations:
Bridging the syntactic cleft.
Psychological Review
99:150–71.
Branigan, Holly P., Martin J. Pickering, Simon P. Liversedge, Andrew J. Stewart, and Thomas P. Urbach.
1995. Syntactic priming: Investigating the mental representation of language.
Journal of
Psycholinguistic Research
24:489–506.
Branigan, Holly P., Martin J. Pickering, Andrew J. Stewart, and Janet F. McLean. 2000. Syntactic priming
in spoken production: Linguistic and temporal interference.
Memory and Cognition
28, no. 8:1297–
1302.
Chang, Franklin, Kathryn Bock, and Adele E. Goldberg. 2003. Do thematic roles leave traces in their
places?
Cognition
90, no. 1:29–49.
Chang, Franklin, Garry S. Dell, Kathryn Bock, and Zenzi M. Griffin. 2000. Structural priming as implicit
learning: A comparison of models of sentence production.
Journal of Psycholinguistic Research
29:217–29.
Chang, Nancy C., and Tiago V. Maia. 2001. Learning grammatical constructions.
Proceedings of the 24th
Annual Cognitive Science Society
. Mahwah, N.J.: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Childers, Jane B., and Michael Tomasello. 2001. The role of pronouns in young children’s acquisition of
the English transitive construction.
Developmental Psychology
37:739–48.
Chomsky, Noam. 1981.
Lectures on government and binding
. Dordrecht, The Netherlands: Foris
Publications.
Croft, William. 2001.
Radical construction grammar
. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Diessel, Holger, and Michael Tomasello. 2001. The acquisition of finite complement clauses in English: A
usage based approach to the development of grammatical constructions.
Cognitive Linguistics
12:97–141.
Fillmore, Charles J. 1968. The case for case. In
Universals in linguistic theory
, ed. Emmon Bach and Rob-
ert T. Harms, 1–88. New York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston.
Garnsey, Susan, Neal J. Pearlmutter, Elizabeth Myers, and Melanie A. Lotocky. 1997. The contributions of
verb bias and plausibility to the comprehension of temporarily ambiguous sentences.
Journal of
Memory and Language
37:58–93.
16
Do'stlaringiz bilan baham: