279
Members of the Trukese tribe in the South Pacifi c often sail
a hundred miles in open
ocean waters. Although their destination may be just a small dot of land less than a mile
wide, the Trukese are able to navigate precisely toward it without the aid of a compass,
chronometer, sextant, or any of the other sailing tools that are used by Western naviga-
tors. They are able to sail accurately even when the winds do not allow a direct approach
to the island and they must take a zigzag course. (Gladwin, 1964; Mytinger, 2001)
How are the Trukese able to navigate so effectively? If you asked them, they could
not explain it. They might tell you that they use a process
that takes into account the
rising and setting of the stars and the appearance, sound, and feel of the waves
against the side of the boat. But at any given moment as they are sailing along, they
could not identify their position or say why they are doing what they are doing. Nor
could they explain the navigational theory underlying their sailing technique.
Some people might say that the inability of the Trukese to explain in Western
terms how their sailing technique works is a sign of primitive or even unintelligent
behavior. In fact, if we gave Trukese sailors a Western standardized test of naviga-
tional knowledge and theory or,
for that matter, a traditional test of intelligence, they
might do poorly on it. Yet, as a practical matter, it is not possible to accuse the Trukese
of being unintelligent: Despite their inability to explain how they do it, they are able
to navigate successfully through the open ocean waters.
Trukese navigation points out the diffi culty in coming to grips with what is
meant by intelligence. To a Westerner, traveling in a straight
line along the most
direct and quickest route by using a sextant and other navigational tools is likely to
represent the most “intelligent” kind of behavior; in contrast, a zigzag course, based
on the “feel” of the waves, would not seem very reasonable. To the Trukese, who
are used to
their own system of navigation, however, the
use of complicated navigational tools might seem so overly
complex and unnecessary that they might think of Western
navigators as lacking in intelligence.
It is clear from this example that the term
intelligence can
take on many different meanings. If, for instance, you lived
in a remote part of the Australian outback, the way you
would differentiate between more
intelligent and less intel-
ligent people might have to do with successfully mastering
hunting skills, whereas to someone living in the heart of
urban Miami, intelligence might be exemplifi ed by being
“streetwise” or by achieving success in business.
Each of these conceptions of intelligence is reasonable.
Each represents an instance in which more intelligent people
are better able to use the resources of their environment than
are less intelligent people, a distinction that is presumably
basic to any defi nition of intelligence. Yet
it is also clear that
these conceptions represent very different views of intelligence.
That two such different sets of behavior can exemplify
the same psychological concept has long posed a challenge
Do'stlaringiz bilan baham: