Applying Psychology
in the 21st Century
Testing the Value
of Self-Affi
rmations:
Am I Lovable Because
I Tell Myself I’m Lovable?
If you’ve ever felt down about yourself,
you might have sought comfort from a
friend or family member who spent some
time telling you what a great and wonder-
ful person you really are. Perhaps you’ve
done the same for a friend of yours who
was having a crisis of confi dence. Pop psy-
chology wisdom tells us that these affi rma-
tive statements, or affi rmations, help us feel
better about ourselves. We’re sometimes
even advised to use self-affi rmations to
help boost our sense of self worth, and it
makes good common sense that if you keep
repeating to yourself statements such as
“I’m a lovable person” or “I will succeed,”
you’ll believe it and like yourself more. But
is that really the case? (Gordon, 2001)
Relying on intuition alone to answer this
question is not enough. Something that
sounds like it should be true may not be.
The only way to tell is to put the hypothesis
to the test—and that’s exactly what a group
of psychology researchers did with affi rma-
tions. They brought several dozen college
students into their laboratory, where they
fi rst asked students to assess their self-
esteem level and mood using question-
naires. Then the researchers instructed the
students to write down their thoughts and
feelings for several minutes. Half the stu-
dents were given an additional instruction:
Whenever a certain tone sounded, they
were to give themselves a self-affi rmation,
telling themselves “I’m a lovable person.”
(The group providing themselves the self-
affi rmation was the experimental group.
The other group, which did not receive this
additional instruction and did not engage
in any self-affi rmations, was the control
group (Wood, Perunovic, & Lee, 2009).
After several minutes (and 16 repeti-
tions of the self-affi rmation for the experi-
mental group), both groups were then
given new tests of their current level of
self esteem and mood to see if changes
occurred. The results clearly showed that
a change did occur for students who had
low self-esteem and who engaged in self-
affi rmations: their mood and self-esteem
went
down. This is, of course, the exact
opposite of the result that proponents of
self-affi rmation would intuitively expect.
Instead, the study showed that not only
were self-affi rmations not helpful but that
they actually seemed to backfi re.
Why might this happen? Interpreting
these surprising fi ndings is easier if we con-
sider the fi ndings of other, related research.
If you consider these self-affi rmations to be
a form of self-persuasion, we can look at
research on persuasion and fi nd that people
tend to be more persuaded by messages
that are in line with their pre- existing atti-
tudes. When a message is strongly contra-
dictory to what a person already believes,
he or she may counter that message with an
argument.
So when people who have low self-
esteem tell themselves a contradictory
message, such as “I’m a lovable person,”
instead of being able to accept the message,
they might think of times when they were
not lovable because they acted unpleas-
antly or were rejected by others. The self-
affi rmation might therefore backfi re by
causing people with low-self-esteem to lin-
ger on negative thoughts that just confi rm
their poor self-image.
These
fi ndings, which run counter to
common-sense explanations, underscore
the need to submit hypotheses to scientifi c
testing. Even when the outcome seems
“obvious,” we need to do research, because
our intuitions are often simply wrong
(Eagly & Chaiken, 1993).
• Are you convinced by the experiment results that self-affi rmations are not effective?
Why or why not?
• Could you design another experiment to test the same hypothesis?
Do'stlaringiz bilan baham: