participants had learned the sequence in the morning and were tested later that
evening after remaining awake across the day. The other half of the subjects
learned the sequence in the evening and we retested them the next morning after
a similar twelve-hour delay, but one that contained a full eight-hour night of sleep.
Those who remained awake across the day showed no evidence of a significant
improvement in performance. However, fitting with the pianist’s original
description, those who were tested after the very same time delay of twelve hours,
but that spanned a night of sleep, showed a striking 20 percent jump in
performance speed and a near 35 percent improvement in accuracy. Importantly,
those participants who learned the motor skill in the morning—and who showed
no improvement that evening—did go on to show an identical bump up in
performance when retested after a further twelve hours, now after they, too, had
had a full night’s sleep.
In other words, your brain will continue to improve skill memories in the
absence of any further practice. It is really quite magical. Yet, that delayed,
“offline” learning occurs exclusively across a period of sleep, and not across
equivalent time periods spent awake, regardless of whether the time awake or
time asleep comes first. Practice does not make perfect. It is practice, followed by
a night of sleep, that leads to perfection. We went on to show that these memory-
boosting benefits occur no matter whether you learn a short or a very long motor
sequence (e.g., 4-3-1-2 versus 4-2-3-4-2-3-1-4-3-4-1-4), or when using one hand
(unimanual) or both (bimanual, similar to a pianist).
Analyzing the individual elements of the motor sequence, such as 4-1-3-2-4,
allowed me to discover how, precisely, sleep was perfecting skill. Even after a long
period of initial training, participants would consistently struggle with particular
transitions within the sequence. These problem points stuck out like a sore
thumb when I looked at the speed of the keystrokes. There would be a far longer
pause, or consistent error, at specific transitions. For example, rather than
seamlessly typing 4-1-3-2-4, 4-1-3-2-4, a participant would instead type: 4-1-3
[pause] 2-4, 4-1-3 [pause] 2-4. They were chunking the motor routine into pieces,
as if attempting the sequences all in one go was just too much. Different people
had different pause problems at different points in the routine, but almost all
people had one or two of these difficulties. I assessed so many participants that I
could actually tell where their unique difficulties in the motor routine were just by
listening to their typing during training.
When I tested participants after a night of sleep, however, my ears heard
something very different. I knew what was happening even before I analyzed the
data: mastery. Their typing, post-sleep, was now fluid and unbroken. Gone was
the staccato performance, replaced by seamless automaticity, which is the
ultimate goal of motor learning: 4-1-3-2-4, 4-1-3-2-4, 4-1-3-2-4, rapid and nearly
perfect. Sleep had systematically identified where the difficult transitions were in
the motor memory and smoothed them out. This finding rekindled the words of
the pianist I’d met: “but when I wake up the next morning and sit back down at
the piano, I can just play, perfectly.”
I went on to test participants inside a brain scanner after they had slept, and
could see how this delightful skill benefit had been achieved. Sleep had again
transferred the memories, but the results were different from that for textbook-
like memory. Rather than a transfer from short- to long-term memory required for
saving facts, the motor memories had been shifted over to brain circuits that
operate below the level of consciousness. As a result, those skill actions were now
instinctual habits. They flowed out of the body with ease, rather than feeling
effortful and deliberate. Which is to say that sleep helped the brain automate the
movement routines, making them second nature—effortless—precisely the goal
of many an Olympic coach when perfecting the skills of their elite athletes.
My final discovery, in what spanned almost a decade of research, identified the
type of sleep responsible for the overnight motor-skill enhancement, carrying
with it societal and medical lessons. The increases in speed and accuracy,
underpinned by efficient automaticity, were directly related to the amount of
stage 2 NREM, especially in the last two hours of an eight-hour night of sleep (e.g.,
from five to seven a.m., should you have fallen asleep at eleven p.m.). Indeed, it
was the number of those wonderful sleep spindles in the last two hours of the late
morning—the time of night with the richest spindle bursts of brainwave activity
—that were linked with the offline memory boost.
More striking was the fact that the increase of these spindles after learning
was detected only in regions of the scalp that sit above the motor cortex (just in
front of the crown of your head), and not in other areas. The greater the local
increase in sleep spindles over the part of the brain we had forced to learn the
motor skill exhaustively, the better the performance upon awakening. Many other
groups have found a similar “local-sleep”-and-learning effect. When it comes to
motor-skill memories, the brainwaves of sleep were acting like a good masseuse
—you still get a full body massage, but they will place special focus on areas of the
body that need the most help. In the same way, sleep spindles bathe all parts of
the brain, but a disproportionate emphasis will be placed on those parts of the
brain that have been worked hardest with learning during the day.
Perhaps more relevant to the modern world is the time-of-night effect we
discovered. Those last two hours of sleep are precisely the window that many of
us feel it is okay to cut short to get a jump start on the day. As a result, we miss
out on this feast of late-morning sleep spindles. It also brings to mind the
prototypical Olympic coach who stoically has her athletes practicing late into the
day, only to have them wake in the early hours of the morning and return to
practice. In doing so, coaches may be innocently but effectively denying an
important phase of motor memory development within the brain—one that fine-
tunes skilled athletic performance. When you consider that very small
performance differences often separate winning a gold medal from a last-place
finish in professional athletics, then any competitive advantage you can gain,
such as that naturally offered by sleep, can help determine whether or not you will
hear your national anthem echo around the stadium. Not without putting too fine
a point on it, if you don’t snooze, you lose.
The 100-meter sprint superstar Usain Bolt has, on many occasions, taken naps
in the hours before breaking the world record, and before Olympic finals in which
he won gold. Our own studies support his wisdom: daytime naps that contain
sufficient numbers of sleep spindles also offer significant motor skill memory
improvement, together with a restoring benefit on perceived energy and reduced
muscle fatigue.
In the years since our discovery, numerous studies have shown that sleep
improves the motor skills of junior, amateur, and elite athletes across sports as
diverse as tennis, basketball, football, soccer, and rowing. So much so that, in
2015, the International Olympic Committee published a consensus statement
highlighting the critical importance of, and essential need for, sleep in athletic
development across all sports for men and women.
X
Professional sports teams are taking note, and for good reason. I have recently
given presentations to a number of national basketball and football teams in the
United States, and for the latter, the United Kingdom. Standing in front of the
manager, staff, and players, I tell them about one of the most sophisticated,
potent, and powerful—not to mention legal—performance enhancers that has
real game-winning potential: sleep.
I back up these claims with examples from the more than 750 scientific studies
that have investigated the relationship between sleep and human performance,
many of which have studied professional and elite athletes specifically. Obtain
anything less than eight hours of sleep a night, and especially less than six hours a
night, and the following happens: time to physical exhaustion drops by 10 to 30
percent, and aerobic output is significantly reduced. Similar impairments are
observed in limb extension force and vertical jump height, together with
decreases in peak and sustained muscle strength. Add to this marked
impairments in cardiovascular, metabolic, and respiratory capabilities that
hamper an underslept body, including faster rates of lactic acid buildup,
reductions in blood oxygen saturation, and converse increases in blood carbon
dioxide, due in part to a reduction in the amount of air that the lungs can expire.
Even the ability of the body to cool itself during physical exertion through
sweating—a critical part of peak performance—is impaired by sleep loss.
And then there is injury risk. It is the greatest fear of all competitive athletes
and their coaches. Concern also comes from the general managers of professional
teams, who consider their players as prized financial investments. In the context
of injury, there is no better risk-mitigating insurance policy for these investments
than sleep. Described in a research study of competitive young athletes in 2014,
XI
you can see that a chronic lack of sleep across the season predicted a massively
higher risk of injury (figure 10).
Do'stlaringiz bilan baham: |