dispersal hypothesis. Contrary to Gimbutas, Renfrew argued that the orig-
inal speakers of PIE were not warriors but farmers, and that the spread of
farming from Anatolia (Turkey) to Greece
and eventually Europe was
responsible for the spread of PIE. This hypothesis leads to a much earlier
dating of the origins of PIE to
c. 10,500 years ago. One of the problems with
this hypothesis for historical linguists, Renfrew (2000: 14) acknowledges,
is that “they assume some specific chronological threshold beyond which
the techniques of the comparative method cannot penetrate.” In other
words, Renfrew’s dates for the origins of PIE extend beyond those for
which linguistic reconstruction can be reliably conducted and point to
the limitations of the comparative method.
The comparative method has clearly yielded valuable information
about languages and the extent to which they are related or unrelated.
However,
this method has limitations, particularly with respect to how far
one can go back in time in the process of reconstructing languages and
language families. We have clear evidence that the Germanic branch of
Indo-European existed, and by examining languages grouped within the
language family, we can infer the existence of Proto-Indo-European. But
some linguists have attempted to go back further in time in the search for
ancestral languages to find, for instance, a larger super-family that would
include Proto-Indo-European. This process involves reconstructing a proto-
language on the basis of other proto-languages that in turn may them-
selves have been reconstructed from proto-languages.
While some lin-
guists have argued that such a process is reliable, others have claimed that
vocabulary, for instance, changes so quickly that this endless process of
reconstruction is fraught with problems.
Greenberg (2000) has proposed a language family called Eurasiatic, which
includes language families such as Indo-European, Uralic, and Altaic as well
as other languages, such as Japanese and Korean, which have defied easy
classification into the major existent language families.
Eurasiatic dates
back to
c. 15,000 years ago and was reconstructed using a method called
mass lexical comparison. This method involves comparing sound similari-
ties between a set of common words in hundreds of languages. Statistical
tests are then conducted to determine the statistical probability that the
languages being compared are related. Another earlier language family that
has been proposed is called Nostratic. Some view this family as an alterna-
tive to Eurasiatic, others as a family that would include Eurasiatic. Still oth-
ers believe in the notion of monogenesis: the idea, as Trask (1996: 391)
observes, “that human language evolved only once,
and that all languages
that have ever been spoken are descended from that single ancestor.” This
original language has been called Proto-World.
The problem with positing language families such as Eurasiatic and
Nostratic is that the reliability of one’s reconstruction diminishes consid-
erably if a hypothetical language family is reconstructed from other lan-
guage families. Moreover, if such reconstructions are based on compar-
isons of vocabulary, it is crucial that these comparisons be based on cog-
nate words, not borrowings. And in many cases it is difficult to determine
whether a given word in a language is a cognate or a borrowing. For these
30
INTRODUCING ENGLISH LINGUISTICS
reasons, many linguists remain skeptical of
reconstructed language fami-
lies such as Eurasiatic and Nostratic.
While it may be difficult to precisely date the origins of PIE and Proto-
Germanic, we can be much more confident about the external history of
English. And knowledge of this history can be combined with surviving
linguistic evidence to provide a fairly precise description of the history of
the English language, and its various stages of development.
Do'stlaringiz bilan baham: