At first, scientists had no idea what the default network did. They had a long list of tasks
that turned it off (telling them what it didn’t do), but little hard evidence about its true
purpose. Even without good evidence, however, scientists began to develop intuitions
based on their own experience. One of these pioneering thinkers is our guide to this
research, Matthew Lieberman—who now enters our narrative as an active participant.
As Lieberman recalls, images of the default network were typically produced by asking a
subject in a PET scanner to take a break from whatever repetitive
activity the experiment
required. Because the subject wasn’t engaged in a specific task, it was easy for researchers
to think of the default network as something that comes on when you’re thinking about
nothing. A little self-reflection, however, makes clear that our brains are hardly ever
actually thinking about nothing. Even without a specific task, they tend to remain highly
active, with thoughts and ideas flitting by in an ongoing noisy chatter. On further self-
reflection, Lieberman realized that this background hum of activity tends to focus on a
small number of targets: thoughts about “other people, yourself, or both.” The default
network, in other words, seems to be connected to
social cognition.
Sure enough, once scientists knew what to look for, they discovered that the regions of
the brain that defined the default network are “virtually identical”
to the networks that
light up during social cognition experiments. When given downtime, in other words, our
brain defaults to thinking about our social life.
It’s here that Lieberman’s research takes an interesting twist. When he first encountered
the conclusion that the default network is social, he wasn’t impressed. Like others in his
field, he noted that people naturally have a strong interest in their own social life, so it’s not
surprising that this is what they like to think about when bored. As Lieberman continued to
study different aspects of social cognition, however, his opinion shifted. “I have since
become convinced that I had the relationship between these networks backward,” he
writes. “And this reversal is tremendously important.” He now believes “we are interested
in the social world
because we are built to turn on the default network during our free
time.” Put another way, our brains adapted to automatically practice social thinking during
any moments of cognitive downtime, and it’s this practice
that helps us become really
interested in our social world.
Lieberman and his collaborators devised a clever series of experiments to confirm this
hypothesis. In one study, for example, they found that the default network lights up during
downtime even in newborns. The importance of finding this activity in infants is that they
“clearly haven’t cultivated an interest in the social world yet. . . . [The infants studied]
cannot even focus their eyes.” This behavior must therefore be instinctual.
In another study, researchers put (adult) subjects in a scanner and asked them to solve
math problems. They discovered that when they gave the subjects a three-second break
between problems—a duration too short for them to decide to start thinking about
something else—the default network still fired up to fill the small gap, further indicating
that this drive to think about social issues kicks in like a reflex.
This finding underscores the fundamental importance of social connections to human
well-being. As Lieberman summarizes: “The brain did not evolve over millions of years to
spend its free time practicing something irrelevant to our lives.” But the default network is
not the whole story. Additional studies by Lieberman and his collaborators uncovered
other examples where evolution placed “big bets” on the importance of sociality by
adapting other expensive systems to serve its needs.
The
loss of social connection, for example, turns out to trigger the same system as
physical pain—explaining why the death of a family member, a breakup, or even just a
social snub can cause such distress. In one simple experiment, it was discovered that over-
the-counter painkillers reduced social pain. Given the power of the pain system in driving
our behavior, its connection to our social life underscores the importance of social
relationships to our species’ success.
Lieberman also discovered that the human brain devotes significant resources to two
different major networks that work together toward the goal of
mentalizing: helping us
understand other people’s minds, including how they are feeling and their intentions.
Something as simple as a casual conversation with a store clerk requires massive amounts
of neuronal computational power to take in and process a high-bandwidth stream of clues
about what’s going on in the clerk’s mind. Though this “mind reading” feels natural to us,
it’s actually an amazingly complicated feat performed by networks
honed over millions of
years of evolution. It’s exactly these highly adapted systems that are leveraged by the rock
paper scissor champions who opened this chapter.
These experiments represent only some key highlights among many from a vast social
cognitive neuroscience literature that all point to the same conclusion: humans are wired
to be social. In other words, Aristotle was on the right track when he called us social
animals, but it took the modern invention of advanced brain scanners to help us figure out
how much he was likely understating this reality.
■
■
■
This highly adapted human interest in social connection is a fascinating piece of our
evolutionary history. It’s also, however, a reality that should concern any digital
minimalist. The intricate brain networks described above evolved over millions of years in
environments where interactions were always rich, face-to-face encounters, and social
groups were small and tribal. The past two decades, by contrast, are characterized by the
rapid spread of
digital communication tools—my name for apps, services, or sites that
enable people to interact through digital networks—which have pushed people’s social
networks to be much larger and much less local, while encouraging interactions through
short, text-based messages and approval clicks that are orders of magnitude less
information laden than what we have evolved to expect.
Perhaps predictably, this clash of old neural systems with modern innovations has
caused problems. Much in the same way that the “innovation” of highly processed foods in
the mid-twentieth century led to a global health crisis, the unintended side effects of digital
communication tools—a sort of social fast food—are proving to be similarly worrisome.
THE SOCIAL MEDIA PARADOX
Determining the impact of digital communication tools on our psychological well-being is
complicated. There’s no shortage of scientific studies examining this topic, but different
groups draw different conclusions from the same literature.
Consider two contrasting takes on this topic that were both published around the same
time in 2017. The first was an NPR story
posted in March of that year, which summarized
the results of a pair of big-deal new studies about the connection between social media use
and well-being. Both studies found strong correlations between social media use and a
range of negative factors, from perceived isolation to poorer physical health. The NPR
story’s title summarizes these findings well: “Feeling Lonely? Too Much Time on Social
Media Might Be Why.”
Not long after this NPR article came out, two members of Facebook’s internal research
team published a blog post defending their service against a rising tide of criticism that had
begun in the aftermath of the contentious 2016 election. In this post, the authors
acknowledge that some uses of social media might make people less happy, but then point
to several research studies that establish that “when used properly,” these services make
subjects measurably happier. Using Facebook to keep in touch with friends and loved ones,
the
authors note, “brings us joy and strengthens our sense of community.”
In other words, depending on whom you ask, social media is either making us lonely or
bringing us joy.
To better understand this general phenomenon of contrasting conclusions, let’s look
closer at the specific studies summarized above. One of the main positive articles cited by
the Facebook blog post was authored by Moira Burke, a data scientist at the company (who