The Evolution of Forensic Science
47
In 1995, the International Fingerprint community endorsed this standard in
the
Ne’urim Declaration
(Israel National Police, 1995; Cole, 1999).
In other words, the decision to “make” a print
has been left entirely up
to the experience and intuition of the examiner.*
In the first edition of
Crime Investigation
(1953), Kirk, in an attempt to
explain the probabilistic nature of fingerprint evidence, gives an example of
how individual fingerprint traits may be multiplied to produce a composite
frequency.
Assume that, on the average, one person in every
twenty is found to have
a whorl on the left thumb. Assume further that one in every ten is found
to have a whorl on the right index finger. Then the probability that an
individual picked at random will have both whorls is 1/20 times 1/10, or
1/200. To generalize, the probability of any combination of characteristics
being found in a given person is the product of the probabilities of each of
the individual characteristics. With the ordinary methods of classifying
fingerprints, the probability of any but the one person in question having
a particular fingerprint classification is so minute as to be negligible. For
this reason only, fingerprints may be accepted as definite identification.
The problem, which Kirk sidesteps along with everyone else, is
the lack of
scientific basis for the actual frequencies of the individual traits; Kirk gives
no foundation for his 1/20 or 1/10 assumption** and fails to even address
the issue of independence. Perhaps it is telling that, although his premise
remains the same in the second edition, edited by John Thornton, the specific
example has disappeared.
In 1986, Stoney and Thornton (1986b) published
an article in which
they reviewed fingerprint individuality models proposed throughout history
and defined a list of features that they resolved should be incorporated in a
comprehensive and utilitarian model. They conclude with the caution that
the value of any fingerprint for identification is inversely proportional to the
chance of false association and that this chance depends on the number of
comparisons that are attempted. These ideas have yet to be developed and
put to practical use.
One consequence of the growing emphasis on individualization has been
the tendency to overinterpret evidence that does
not necessarily hold indi-
vidualizing potential. The history of microscopic hair comparison exemplifies
* It should be noted that the IAI resolution is not binding and many agencies continue to
adhere to a minimum number of corresponding points to conclude identity. For instance,
Great Britain continues to hold tenaciously to a standard of 16 points.
** Some basis for these estimates may be found in historical fingerprint models such as
that of Gupta (Stoney and Thornton, 1986a), but Kirk provides no reference.
8127/frame/ch02 Page 47 Friday, July 21, 2000 11:51 AM