1 8 4
T H E W A R R E N B U F F E T T W AY
pneumonia with 90 percent confidence when in fact they are right only
50 percent of the time.
Conf idence per se is not a bad thing. But
over
conf idence is an-
other matter, and it can be particularly damaging when we are dealing
with our f inancial affairs. Overconf ident investors not only make silly
decisions for themselves but also have a powerful effect on the market
as a whole.
Overconf idence explains why so many investors make wrong calls.
They have too much conf idence in the
information they gather and
think they are more right than they actually are. If all the players think
that their information is correct and they know something that others
do not, the result is a great deal of trading.
Overreaction Bias
Thaler points to several studies that demonstrate people put too much
emphasis on a few chance events, thinking they spot a trend. In partic-
ular, investors tend to f ix on the most recent information they received
and extrapolate from it; the last earnings report thus becomes in their
mind a signal of future earnings. Then, believing that they see what
others do not, they make quick decisions from superf icial reasoning.
Overconfidence
is at work here; people believe they understand the
data more clearly than others do and interpret it better. But there is more
to it. Overreaction exacerbates overconfidence. The behaviorists have
learned that people tend to overreact to bad news and react slowly to good
news. Psychologists call this
overreaction bias.
Thus if the short-term
earnings report is not good, the typical
investor response is an abrupt, ill-
considered overreaction, with its inevitable effect on stock prices.
Thaler describes this overemphasis on the short term as investor
“myopia” (the medical term for nearsightedness) and believes most in-
vestors would be better off if they didn’t receive monthly statements. In
a study conducted with other behavioral economists, he proved his idea
in dramatic fashion.
Thaler and colleagues asked a group of students to divide a hypo-
thetical portfolio between stocks and Treasury bills. But f irst, they sat
the students in front of a computer and simulated the returns of the
portfolio over a trailing twenty-f ive-year period.
Half the students
were given mountains of information, representing the market’s
volatile nature with ever-changing prices. The other group was only
T h e P s y c h o l o g y o f M o n e y
1 8 5
given periodic performance measured in five-year time periods. Thaler
then asked each group to allocate their portfolio for the next forty years.
The group that had been bombarded by lots of information, some of
which inevitably pointed to losses, allocated only 40 percent of its
money
to the stock market; the group that received only periodic infor-
mation allocated almost 70 percent of its portfolio to stocks. Thaler,
who lectures each year at the Behavioral Conference sponsored by the
National Bureau of Economic Research and the John F. Kennedy School
of Government at Harvard, told the group, “My advice to you is to in-
vest in equities and then don’t open the mail.”
11
This experiment, as well as others, neatly underscores Thaler’s no-
tion of investor myopia—shortsightedness leading to foolish decisions.
Do'stlaringiz bilan baham: