P B S F O X C N N A B C C B S M T V N B C
chunk
A meaningful grouping of
stimuli that can be stored as a unit in
short-term memory.
A momentary fl ash of lightning leaves a
sensory visual memory, a fl eeting but
exact replica of the stimulus that fades
away.
feL82795_ch07_206-239.indd Page 211 7/29/10 9:46 PM user-f465
feL82795_ch07_206-239.indd Page 211 7/29/10 9:46 PM user-f465
/Users/user-f465/Desktop
/Users/user-f465/Desktop
212 Chapter
7
Memory
Because the list exceeds seven chunks, it is diffi cult to recall the letters after one
exposure. But suppose they were presented as follows:
PBS FOX CNN ABC CBS MTV NBC
In this case, even though there are still 21 letters, you’d be able to store them in
short-term memory since they represent only seven chunks.
Chunks can vary in size from single letters or numbers to categories that are far
more complicated. The specifi c nature of what constitutes a chunk varies according
to one’s past experience. You can see this for yourself by trying an experiment that
was fi rst carried out as a comparison between expert and inexperienced chess play-
ers and is illustrated in Figure 3 (deGroot, 1978; Oberauer, 2007; Gilchrist, Cowan, &
Naveh-Benjamin, 2009).
Although it is possible to remember seven or so relatively complicated sets of
information entering short-term memory, the information cannot be held there very
long. Just how brief is short-term memory? If you’ve ever looked up a telephone
number in a phone directory, repeated the number to yourself, put away the direc-
tory, and then forgotten the number after you’ve tapped the fi rst three numbers into
your phone, you know that information does not remain in short-term memory very
long. Most psychologists believe that information in short-term memory is lost after
15 to 25 seconds—unless it is transferred to long-term memory.
FIGURE 3
Examine the chessboard on the left for about fi ve seconds. Then cover up the
board and draw the position of the pieces on the blank chessboard. (You could also use a
chessboard of your own and place the pieces in the same positions.) Unless you are an
experienced chess player, you are likely to have great diffi
culty carrying out such a task. Yet
chess masters—those who win tournaments—do this quite well (deGroot, 1966). They are
able to reproduce correctly 90% of the pieces on the board. In comparison, inexperienced
chess players are typically able to reproduce only 40% of the board properly. The chess
masters do not have superior memories in other respects; they generally test normally on
other measures of memory. What they can do better than others is see the board in terms
of chunks or meaningful units and reproduce the position of the chess pieces by using
those units.
feL82795_ch07_206-239.indd Page 212 7/29/10 9:46 PM user-f465
feL82795_ch07_206-239.indd Page 212 7/29/10 9:46 PM user-f465
/Users/user-f465/Desktop
/Users/user-f465/Desktop
Do'stlaringiz bilan baham: |