202 Chapter
6
Learning
viewed more than 8,000 murders and more than 800,000 violent acts on
network television (Huston et al., 1992; Miffl in, 1998).
Most experts agree that watching high levels of media violence makes
viewers more susceptible to acting aggressively, and recent research sup-
ports this claim (Boxer et al., 2009; Carnagey, Anderson, & Bartholow,
2007; Savage & Yancey, 2008). For example, one survey of serious and
violent young male offenders incarcerated in Florida showed that one-
fourth of them had attempted to commit a media-inspired copycat crime
(Surette, 2002). A signifi cant proportion of those teenage offenders noted
that they paid close attention to the media.
Violent video games have also been linked with actual aggression. In
one of a series of studies by psychologist Craig Anderson and his col-
leagues, for example, college students who frequently played violent
video games, such as
Postal or
Doom , were more likely to have been
involved in delinquent behavior and aggression. Frequent players also
had lower academic achievement (Anderson et al., 2004; Anderson & Car-
nagey, 2009; Swing & Anderson, 2007).
Several aspects of media violence may contribute to real-life aggressive
behavior (Bushman & Anderson, 2001; Johnson et al., 2002). For one thing,
experiencing violent media content seems to lower inhibitions against car-
rying out aggression—watching television portrayals of violence or using
violence to win a video game makes aggression seem a legitimate response
to particular situations. Exposure to media violence also may distort our understand-
ing of the meaning of others’ behavior, predisposing us to view even nonaggressive
acts by others as aggressive. Finally, a continuous diet of aggression may leave us
desensitized to violence, and what previously would have repelled us now produces
little emotional response. Our sense of the pain and suffering brought about by aggres-
sion may be diminished (Bartholow, Bushman, & Sestir, 2006; Carnagey, Anderson,
& Bushman, 2007; Weber, Ritterfeld, & Kostygina, 2006).
What about real-life exposure to
actual violence? Does it also lead to increases in
aggression? The answer is yes. Exposure to actual fi rearm violence (being shot or
being shot at) doubles the probability that an adolescent will commit serious violence
over the next two years. Whether the violence is real or fi ctionalized, then, observing
violent behavior leads to increases in aggressive behavior (Allwood, 2007; Bingen-
heimer, Brennan, & Earls, 2005).
When a member of the Chilcotin Indian tribe teaches her daughter to
prepare salmon, at fi rst she allows the daughter only to observe the
entire process. A little later, she permits her child to try out some basic
Do'stlaringiz bilan baham: