Consciousness is the awareness of the sensations, thoughts,
and feelings we experience at a given moment. Consciousness is
our subjective understanding of both the environment around us
and our private internal world, unobservable to outsiders.
In
waking consciousness
, we are awake and aware of our
thoughts, emotions, and perceptions. All other states of con-
sciousness are considered
altered states of consciousness
. Among
these, sleeping and dreaming occur naturally; drug use and
hypnosis, in contrast, are methods of deliberately altering one’s
state of consciousness.
In the past, because consciousness is so personal a phenom-
enon, psychologists were sometimes reluctant to study it. After
all, who can say that your consciousness is similar to or, for that
matter, diff erent from anyone else’s? Although the earliest
psychologists, including William James (1890), saw the study of
consciousness as central to the fi eld, later psychologists sug-
gested that it was out of bounds for the discipline. They argued
that consciousness could be understood only by relying “unscien-
tifi cally” on what experimental participants said they were
experiencing. In this view, it was philosophers—not psycholo-
gists—who should speculate on such knotty issues as whether
consciousness is separate from the physical body, how people
know they exist, and how the body and mind are related to each
other (Barresi, 2007; Gennaro, 2004; Rychlak, 1997).
Contemporary psychologists reject the view that the study of
consciousness is unsuitable for the fi eld of psychology. Instead,
they argue that several approaches permit the scientifi c study of
consciousness. For example, behavioral neuroscientists can
measure brain-wave patterns under conditions of consciousness
ranging from sleep to waking to hypnotic trances. And new
understanding of the chemistry of drugs such as marijuana and
alcohol has provided insights into the way they produce their
pleasurable—as well as adverse—eff ects (Baars & Seth, 2009;
Damasio, 2003; Mosher & Akins, 2007).
Yet how humans experience consciousness remains an open
question. Some psychologists believe that the experience of
consciousness is produced by a quantitative increase in neuronal
activity that occurs throughout the brain. For example, an alarm
clock moves us from sleep to waking consciousness by its loud
ringing, which stimulates neurons throughout the brain as a
whole (Greenfi eld, 2002; Koch & Greenfi eld, 2007).
In contrast, others believe that states of consciousness are
produced by particular sets of neurons and neuronal pathways
that are activated in specifi c ways. In this view, an alarm clock
wakes us from sleep into consciousness, because specifi c
neurons related to the auditory nerve are activated; the auditory
nerve then sends a message to other neurons to release particu-
lar neurotransmitters that produce awareness of the alarm
(Tononi & Koch, 2008).
Although we don’t know yet which of these views is correct, it
is clear that whatever state of consciousness we are in—be it
waking, sleeping, hypnotic, or drug-induced—the complexities
of consciousness are profound.
Do'stlaringiz bilan baham: |