89
Another significant convention in
literary studies is that of
genre (cf.
Section 6). It is generally assumed that the features of a text are determined by
its particular generic form:
genre focuses on the specific qualities and structures
of the text. Shifting from its descriptive to an explanatory role, the category of
genre “embodies certain social roles that govern the relation between text and
reader” (Miall 2002: 325). Many authors have expressed their views on genre
assuming that “there is no genreless text” (Derrida 1980: 65). In this respect,
genre can be understood as the defining context for all textual behaviour, literary
and non-literary. Therefore we can say that genre determines a speaker’s textual
behaviour in each particular situation, context or sphere. Linguistic variation
which
correlates with situation, social setting and social role is known as
register. Thus we may assume that genre governs register: the category of genre
determines the use of a particular register in any given text, the use of actual
semantic and syntactic features that create the given communicative situation,
together with the speaker’s role and attitude (cf. Halliday 1978). As
discourse
structures genres are characterized by types of story grammar or schemata, they
specify situation models characteristic of a given literary text and they enable
readers to predict how the text is likely to develop. This provides space for more
complex empirical studies on the reading of literary texts, involving a debate
about “whether literary texts enjoy some distinctive status or literariness” (Miall
2002: 326).
The term literariness originally implied that there are literary texts which
show certain distinctiveness from other text types. The study of literariness has
attracted the attention of generations of scholars.
More recently the idea of a
conventional nature of literature has been emphasized and consequently the idea
that literary texts are distinctive by the quality of specific features, dismissed. As
demonstrated in stylistics by extensive analytical work on a variety of text types,
formal aspects of language cannot guarantee stable meaning and no aspects of
language can be labelled as formal (cf. Miššíková 2009, 2011).
In the empirical domain, scholars have studied cognitive processes,
pointing
out that they apply equally to our comprehension of text and refusing the idea
that specific or distinct cognitive processes are in progress when literary text is
interpreted. As emphasized by van Dijk (1979: 151) we have to “strictly deny
the completely ‘specific’ nature of so-called ‘literary interpretation’” and see
differences in terms of pragmatic and social functions of literature.
Do'stlaringiz bilan baham: