(2)
principles stipulating how the structures that rules create should be
used (e.g. which forms will be polite in which contexts, which forms
will not).
Rules are studied under the rubric of
grammar,
principles within the
province of
pragmatics.
To understand what
is meant by rules and prin-
ciples, and why they are studied within grammar and pragmatics, consid-
er why a three-year-old child would utter a sentence such as
I broked it
[ai broυkt
I
t] to his father, who just entered a room
that the child was play-
ing in to discover that the child had broken a wheel off a truck that he had
been playing with.
To account for why the child uttered
I broked it rather than, say,
Breaked
it I, it is necessary to investigate the linguistic rules the child is using to
create the structure that he did. Linguistic rules are different from the
rules that people learn in school: “Don’t
end sentences with preposi-
tions”; “Don’t begin a sentence with
but”; “Don’t split infinitives.” These
are
prescriptive rules (discussed in greater detail in the next section) and
are intended to provide guidance to students as they learn to speak and
write so-called Standard English. Linguistic rules, in contrast, serve to
describe what people know about language:
the unconscious knowledge
of language they possess that is part of what Noam Chomsky describes as
our linguistic competence. Even though the sentence the child uttered
does not conform to the rules of Standard English – the past tense form of
the verb
break is
broke, not
broked – it provides evidence that the child is
aware of the rules of English grammar. He has applied a past tense ending
for
the verb, spelled -
ed in writing, but has not reached a stage of acquisi-
tion where he is able to recognize the difference between regular and
irregular verb forms.
Rules of grammar operate at various levels:
Phonetics/Phonology: This level focuses on the smallest unit of structure in
language, the phoneme. Linguistic rules at this level describe how
sounds are pronounced in various contexts.
For instance, there is a rule
of
voicing assimilation in English that stipulates that when a past
tense marker is added to the stem of a verb, the last sound in the stem
determines
whether the marker is voiced or
unvoiced (i.e. whether or
not the vocal cords vibrate when the consonant is pronounced). Thus,
even though the child uses the wrong past tense form, the past tense
marker is pronounced as /t/ because the last sound in the stem, /k/, is
unvoiced. Had the stem been
kill, which ends in voiced /l/,
the past
tense marker would have been voiced /d/. The sound system of English
and the rules that govern it are discussed in detail in Chapter 7.
Morphology: The next level of structure is the morpheme, the smallest unit
of meaning in language. Rules of morphology focus on how words (and
Do'stlaringiz bilan baham: