‘Strategies’ for Creating New Words
As far as morphological processes in word-formation are concerned, we can distinguish between a variety of major types, briefly introduced and summarised in the table below:
Major types of word formation processes
|
Process
|
Function
|
affixation
|
changing words by adding morphemes in the front or the back of a free morpheme or base; sub-divided into prefigation & suffigation
|
zero-derivation
|
changing the word class without changing the word shape
|
compounding
|
creating new words by combining (mainly) free morphemes
|
backformation
|
creating new words from phrases
|
clipping & blending
|
abbreviating or ‘fusing’ words into new ones.
|
acronym formation
|
using initials to create short words
|
We’ll discuss each of these processes in some detail below.
Affixation:
Inflection vs.
Derivation
Affixation is the general process of attaching bound – rather than free – morphemes to a base. We can sub-divide the morphemes occurring in affixation processes further into the following types, based on their positions of attachement:
prefixes attach at the beginning: {im}+{poss}{ible}, {un}+{able}
suffixes attach at the end: {act}+{ress}, {baron}+{ess}
circumfixes consist of two parts, a prefix and a suffix: Germ. {ge}+{geb}+{en}; both need to attach to create the final form
infixes ‘attach’ (i.e. get inserted) in the middle: {ab}{so+{blood}{y}+lute}{ly}
As we’ve already observed from Old English onwards in our previous examples, prefigation and suffigation (also referred to as prefixation & suffixation) are relatively common and straightforward processes. Especially in Old English, we saw that there were many more suffixes indicating inflectional features of nouns and verbs, such as e.g. case marking with agreement on adjectives and nouns in noun phrases (e.g. (mid) langum sċipum). Many of these have since disappeared from the English language, so that we now only have the limited set of inflectional options left that we noted in our discussion of word classes and their morphology.
In the frequency list for Old English, we were also still able to see examples of the Germanic circumfix, in those word forms that started with {ġe·} and ended in the allomorphs {on} or {an}. In terms of its underlying mechanism, infigation (infixation) – something we did not observe before – is a rather different thing in that it represents a feature that is relatively uncommon, and appears to be restricted solely to creating highly emphatic responses or exclamations that involve swear words.
To fully understand the word formation options affixation covers, we need to distinguish between its two major functions, the inflectional and the derivational one. While inflection, as we have seen, allow us to relate words to one another on the syntagmatic level, i.e. indicating what kinds of roles they perform on the clause level, how they combine with other words, or what kind of tense/aspect they may express, derivation makes it possible to create new words from old ones, either by changing their word class or modifying/‘specifying’ their meanings. In terms of their productivity, inflection has clearly diminished over time, whereas derivation still remains productive.
Although many affixes appear to have a relatively clearly defined function, recognising affix functionality is not always straightforward. Often what may superficially look like a specific affix (or a root) with a certain meaning may either be part of a longer unit, or not constitute an affix at all. Furthermore, we also encounter the same problem we saw earlier on (for instance with the {s} morpheme), i.e. that one and the same form may actually be multi-functional in representing a number of different meanings. In order to be able to understand this problem better, as well as to explore the potential functions of pre- and suffixes, let’s investigate some presumed pre- or suffixes by removing them and observing whether this whether may lead to potential misinterpretations.
This process of stripping off affixes is called stemming and is, these days, not only used in linguistics, but also in IT-related applications, such as search engines or information retrieval programs, in order to derive a root from a complex word form, thereby keeping track of related information without the need to list each word form (or lemma) of a particular paradigm.
The form below allows you to test the effect of stemming for the individual words listed in the dropdown boxes. Simply select a particular word and click on the stem word button. The input word will then be split for you and the two parts appear in the text box on the right, separated by a + symbol.
Try stemming all the different words
Explain the particular function of the suffix (if it is indeed one) that has been split off to the best of your ability. To do so, you need to ‘think backwards’, trying to identify the word class of the original word (i.e. the one before the plus sign in the result), as well as the resulting one (i.e. the one that appears in the dropdown list on the left.
Attempt to find explanations/rules for those cases where the resulting splits do not make sense, either from the point of view of separating the word forms into affix+base, or why the pre-/suffix may sometimes have a different meaning from the one the stemmer is supposed to identify.
Record your observations in the relevant box and save them.
Use appropriate morpheme brackets in your explanations to indicate the parts clearly.
prefix stemmer
As before, split the individual words using the button.
Try to identify the function/meaning of the (potential) prefix as far as possible, or at least how it changes the meaning of the base. If you think it’s not a prefix, justify your opinion.
Check to see whether the result represents an allomorph of another morpheme.
Record and save your observations again.
Reducing a word form too far is referred to as overstemming. This often happens when combinations of letters that look like suffixes are stripped off when the root form has already been reached, or when a change in the combination of letters in the word formation process has been missed by the rules. If you want to try out stemming for other words/suffixes not catered for by the two example programs above, you can test the Porter stemmer, one of the best known and widely used stemming algorithms, mainly used in internet applications, on the Morphology page of my Introduction to Linguistics course.
A mentioned before, the counterpart to reception is production. Production in affixation is characterised by the ability of certain affixes to attach to bases. As you probably already know, not all pre- or suffixes can simply be attached to any base. Let’s test this by trying out different combinations of pre-/suffixes and bases in the form below. If the resulting word form is correct, it will appear in green in the text box on the right, otherwise in red.
Using the form provided below, try out different possibilities for affixation.
See whether you can explain the results in terms of the combinatorial options for different affixes, as well as in terms of productivity.
Also try to see whether it may be possible to identify various allomorphs of the stems listed in the middle.
Different affixes exhibit different characteristics in terms of how frequently they are involved in word formation processes. Some affixes, such as the negation morphemes, are more productive than others
In all the above examples, we’ve created new words or word forms by adding either a pre-, or a suffix, or even both, sometimes also adjusting the stem in order to make things fit together better. However, as we’ve already seen before, sometimes we encounter word forms that can function in different ways and it’s not easy to determine which word class exactly they belong to unless we investigate their morpho-syntactic behaviour. The use of a word form in a word class it ‘doesn’t belong’ to has commonly been assumed to be possible due to a process called zero-derivation or conversion, where a word changes its word class through the addition of a {∅}-suffix.
In many cases, it is the verb that is assumed to be the original form, which then generally ‘turns into’ a noun, as in e.g. I like to run marathons. vs. Let’s go for a run!, I need to think about it. vs. I need to have a (quick) think about it.. At other times, it’s the other way round, for example in There’s a (big) ship in the harbour vs. We need to ship some goods by tomorrow.. Such instances are quite frequent in English, where often verbal word forms can be used as nouns and vice versa.
In rarer cases, we can also have conversion applying to other word classes, such as from adjective to noun, e.g. The poor man had no proper clothes to wear. vs. They gave some money to the poor., or preposition/particle to verb, e.g. Let’s go down to the river! vs. He can down a large beer in only a few seconds.
The decision about which form should be seen as the original one usually seems to be made on the basis of which one of the two forms is more common, but this is really impossible to say without full knowledge of the historical development of the word – and perhaps also unnecessary. What’s far more important to understand in this context is that many words or word forms in English allow us to use them with different morpho-syntactic functions and meanings, although they frequently share some core meaning – in other words, they are simply grammatically polysemous. The notion of core meaning, however, is something that may be difficult to define, as can be seen quite clearly in the form above, where the noun refers to ‘a vessel used to transport items’, and the verb the ‘act of transportation’ itself, although, these days, this act is frequently no longer performed by actually using this type of vessel. On the other hand, though, due to this very fact, we can actually make certain assumptions about the etymology of the verb, and that the noun may have been used first.
How frequent words with 2 or more meanings in fact are in English can be seen from the table below, where the number of word classes is based on an analysis of word-class ambiguity in the 1 million-word BROWN Corpus.
Ambiguity in word classes, based on DeRose (1988)
|
Word Classes
|
Frequency
|
2
|
3,760
|
3
|
264
|
4
|
61
|
5
|
12
|
6
|
2
|
7
|
1 (still)
|
As this is only a fairly small corpus, though, this table may not really tell us the whole story, though, and we can assume that the overall number of grammatically polysemous words may be even higher. Furthermore, if we take into acount that words can also take on a different function without changing their word class, as in examples of compounding we’ve encountered before, and will discuss in more detail in the next section, then this multi-functionality of word forms in English becomes even more striking.
Compounding allows us to create new, and more complex or precise words by combining word forms with lexical meaning. Often, these word forms constitute free morphemes, but, as we’ve seen before, they can also be bound morphemes (e.g. cranberry ones), or other forms that are originally derived from inflected verb forms (e.g. dancing girl or scented oil). We can basically distinguish between three different types of compounds, based on their semantic properties:
Do'stlaringiz bilan baham: |