194
method rather than to the constructs the measures represent. Such a variance may occur as a
result of factors such as social desirability, halo effect and selective
memory brought about
by the self-reporting method, and it can threaten the internal validity of conclusions about the
predictive relationships between measures (Campbell and Fiske 1959, Howard 1994, Spector
1994). As suggested by Kaynak (1997), a researcher therefore should plan how to overcome
common method variance.
As confirmed in the literature, one of the techniques for
minimizing common method
variance is to carefully design the questionnaire and survey procedures.
Specifically,
assurances were given that the data provided by respondents would be held in strict
confidence, the analysis would be done at the aggregate level, and no respondent would be
identified individually. All of this information was stated clearly on the project information
sheet provided to each informant. These procedures also were aimed
at reducing respondents
’
evaluation apprehension, so making them less likely to edit their responses to be more
socially desirable (Gupta and Kim 2007). In addition, the measurement scales in the survey
were arranged so that the measures of independent variables preceded the dependent
variables and items on constructs which have the same scale poles (e.g., TCs,
e-service
quality, etc.) were distributed in a non-sequential order (Salancik and Pfeffer 1977).
In the behavioural sciences, there have been a number of published techniques which assist
with the assessment of common method variance, for example, partial correlation procedures,
Harman
’
s one-factor test,
multiple method factors test, etc. However, no test is without its
disadvantages (Podsakoff et al., 2003).
195
A statistical technique widely used by scholars to determine the influence of common method
variance is Harman
’
s one-factor (or single-factor) test (Podsakoff and Organ 1986).
Researchers using this technique traditionally load all variables in their study into an
exploratory factor analysis (EFA), and examine the unrotated factor solution to determine the
number of factors that are necessary to account for variance in the variables (Organ and
Greene 1981, Andersson and Bateman 1997, Aulakh and Gencturk 2000).
It is assumed that
if only one factor emerges from the unrotated factor solution as accounting for most of the
variance observed in the data, it is likely that common method variance is the primary source
(Podsakoff and Organ 1986).
As an alternative to EFA, confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) can be used when
implementing Harman
’
s single-factor test (Podsakoff and Organ 1986). Specifically, in the
CFA approach, all of the manifested items are modelled as the indicators of a single factor
that represents method effects. Common method bias are assumed to be substantial if the
single-factor model fits the data significantly better than the proposed model with many
factors (Podsakoff and Organ 1986).
In this study, Harman
’
s one-factor test was performed
through both EFA and CFA (reported in the next chapter) in order to detect the severity of
common method variance in the current data.
Do'stlaringiz bilan baham: