Introducing Cognitive Linguistics
Page
14
of
21
PRINTED FROM OXFORD HANDBOOKS ONLINE (www.oxfordhandbooks.com).
©
Oxford University Press, 2022. All Rights
Reserved. Under the terms of the licence agreement, an individual user may print out a PDF of a single chapter of a title in
Oxford Handbooks Online for personal use (for details see
Privacy Policy
and
Legal Notice).
date: 06 June 2022
in corpora) may be determined simultaneously by grammatical, discursive, and sociolin
guistic factors. Disentangling those different factors, then, becomes one methodological
endeavor: in the actual practice of a usage-based enquiry, grammatical analysis and varia
tionist analysis will go hand in hand.
For another, there is an interest in cultural models and the way in which they may com
pete within a community: see, for instance, many of the papers collected in Dirven, Frank,
and Pütz (2003). In work such as Lakoff (1996), this approach takes on a critical aspect
that brings it close to the tradition of ideological analysis known as Critical Discourse
Analysis. Some researchers are applying the theory of conceptual metaphors and cultural
models to questions of social identity and the role language plays in them: see the collec
tive volumes edited by Dirven, Frank, and Ilie (2001), Dirven, Frank, and Putz (2003), and
Dirven, Hawkins, and Sandikcioglu (2001). It has recently been pointed out (Berthele
2001; Geeraerts
2003) that such metaphorical models may also characterize the beliefs
that language users entertain regarding language and language varieties. In this way,
Cognitive Linguistics may link up with existing sociolinguistic research about language
attitudes.
These developments show that the interest in sociovariational analysis in Cognitive Lin
guistics is on the rise, but at the same time, it has to be recognized that the final contex
tual gap that we discussed in the previous section still has to be filled properly.
2. If we understand
empirical methods
to refer to forms of research (like corpus linguis
tics, experimentation, and neurological modeling) that do not rely on introspection and in
tuition but that try to ground linguistic analysis on the firm basis of objective observation,
then we can certainly witness a growing appeal of such empirical methods within Cogni
tive Linguistics: see the argumentation of Gibbs (2006) and Geeraerts (2006b) in favor of
empirical methods, and compare the practical introduction provided by Gonzalez-Mar
quez, Mittelberg, Coulson, and Spivey (2007
). The theoretical background of this develop
ment is provided by the growing tendency of Cognitive Linguistics to stress its essential
nature as a usage-based linguistics—a form of linguistic analysis, that is, that takes into
account not just grammatical structure, but that sees this structure as arising from and
interacting with actual language use. The central notions of usage-based linguistics have
been programmatically outlined in different publications (Langacker
1990; Kemmer and
Barlow
2000; Tomasello
2000,
2003; Bybee and Hopper
2001b; Croft and Cruse
2004),
and a number of recent volumes show how the program can be put into practice (Barlow
and Kemmer
2000; Bybee and Hopper
2001a; Verhagen and van de Weijer
2003). The link
between the self-awareness of Cognitive Linguistics as a usage-based form of linguistic
investigation and the deployment of empirical methods is straightforward: you cannot
have a usage-based linguistics unless you study actual usage—as it appears in corpora in
the form of spontaneous, nonelicited language data or as it appears in an online and
elicited form in experimental settings.
Also, if Cognitive Linguistics belongs to cognitive science, it would be natural to expect
the use of techniques that have proved their value in the cognitive sciences at large. Ex
Do'stlaringiz bilan baham: |