584 Chapter
17
Social
Psychology
that they were about to hear a guest lecturer (Kelley, 1950). Researchers told one
group of students that the lecturer was “a rather warm person, industrious, critical,
practical,
and determined,” and told a second group that he was “a rather cold per-
son, industrious, critical, practical, and determined.”
The simple substitution of “cold” for “warm” caused drastic
differences in the way
the students in each group perceived the lecturer even though he gave the same talk
in the same style in each condition. Students who had been told he was “warm” rated
him considerably more positively than students who had been told he was “cold.”
The fi ndings from this experiment led to additional research on impression for-
mation that focused on the way in which people pay particular attention to certain
unusually important traits—known as
central traits —to
help them form an overall
impression of others. According to this work, the presence of a central trait alters the
meaning of other traits. Hence, the description of the lecturer as “industrious” pre-
sumably meant something different when it was associated with the central trait
“warm” than it meant when it was associated with “cold” (Widmeyer & Loy, 1988;
Glicksohn & Nahari, 2007).
Other work on impression formation has used information-processing approaches
to develop mathematically oriented models of how individual
personality traits com-
bine to create an overall impression. Generally, the results of this research suggest
that in forming an overall judgment of a person, we use a psychological “average”
of the individual traits we see just as we would fi nd the mathematical average of
several numbers (Mignon & Mollaret, 2002).
We make such impressions remarkably quickly.
In just a few seconds, using what
have been called “thin slices of behavior,” we are able to make judgments of people
that are accurate and that match those of people who make judgments based on
longer samples of behavior (Carney, Colvin, & Hall, 2007; Pavitt, 2007; Holleran,
Mehl, & Levitt, 2009).
Of course, as we gain more experience with people
and see them exhibiting
behavior in a variety of situations, our impressions of them become more complex.
However, because our knowledge of others usually has gaps, we still tend to fi t
individuals into personality schemas that represent particular “types” of people. For
instance, we may hold a “gregarious person” schema made up of the traits of friend-
liness, aggressiveness, and openness. The presence of just
one or two of those traits
may be suffi cient to make us assign a person to a particular schema.
However, our schemas are susceptible to error. For example, mood affects how
we perceive others. Happy people form more favorable impressions and make more
positive judgments than people who are in a bad mood (Forgas & Laham, 2005).
Even when schemas are not entirely accurate, they serve an important function:
They allow us to develop expectations about how others will behave. Those expecta-
tions permit us to plan our interactions with others more easily
and serve to simplify
a complex social world.
Do'stlaringiz bilan baham: